As a twenty-five year old director, still finding my feet in the world of drama, I cast the veteran actor, John Welsh, for a cameo role of a man who had seen more than he was willing to tell. I thought I had detected a reticent quality in him, perhaps not common among actors. However, in rehearsal I found that was not coming across
the way that I had hoped. I suggested to him that he should play the scene without looking directly at the other actor in the scene. John looked at me wide-eyed and asked why on earth would he not look at the person to whom he was speaking. I tried to explain that a shy person is often quite fearful of looking another directly in the eye, or meeting the look of another. He shook his head as if he thought that I was quite mad, and then suggested that, as the scene took place in a conservatory, he could be preoccupied with repotting a plant.
John Welsh |
I had given John a direction in the negative mode — not to do something. He had translated this into the positive mode by inventing the business of fiddling with plant pots. He had inserted an action in place of an avoidance. I settled for this even though I felt that it was missing the point. Many years later I saw just what I had been looking for repeated week after week on BBC
television’s The Fast Show, in the Ted & Ralph sketches,
brilliantly played by Paul Whitehouse and Charlie Higson.
In these Ted rarely looks Ralph in the eye because he does not want to face up to the truth of what he secretly knows that he would find there. This is perhaps something that could only really come across on screen and in close-up.
Charlie Higson, Paul Whitehouse as Ted and Ralph in The Fast Show |
Musing on my failure to get this idea across to John Welsh I came to realise that the direction I had given him cut across a long theatrical tradition. One of the primary ways that the stage director can focus audience attention is by having the actor look at the person whom he is addressing and other actors on the stage to follow the conversation with their eyes until it is their turn to speak. We accept this convention without thinking but in reality the play of looks and glances is much richer and more diverse. Though acting for film and TV, supported by camerawork and editing, tends to follow the stage convention, on screen the eyes — set free — may well carry more meaning than the words.
Peter Egan in The Prince Regent |
Peter took a step back and played the speech mainly looking down-right, with quick looks up to his interlocutor at the end of lines. The delivery was natural enough but what he had done without thinking was take up the stock position of the opened-out two-shot — just what I had set out to avoid. This is a position which originally came from stage convention, where two actors in dialogue, instead of facing up, would stand at ninety degrees to each other so that the audience could see their faces. Later it was adopted in film, and especially television, to allow two cameras to simultaneously shoot over-shoulder shots of the two actors without getting in each other’s picture. It was a setup which I referred to as the “cocktail party stance”, because the only people who ever stood this way in real life were guests at a cocktail party who were more interested in looking round the room than the person to whom they were speaking.
one of the many double closeups in Ingmar Bergman's Persona |
It is an interesting discovery to make that when we are not looking at the person with whom we are speaking our eyes betray more of what is going on in our mind than when we do. This is known in Neuro-linguistic Programming as the Eye-accessing Cues”. Once you have become aware of these movements they are so obvious that it is astonishing that they were
not formally recognised until the nineteen seventies. Some of the more obvious cues were, of course, instinctively known to actors; such as the flirtatious look held a beat too long followed by the lowering eyes down right to access feelings. Or, the guilty sideways look right when an alibi is broken. the point is that if an actor is experiencing his role, and not just going through the motions, when he is not looking directly at the person he is addressing his eyes are by no means dead, but just the opposite.
NLP Eye Accessing Cues |
In a masterclass for BBC Television, that was much-talked about by actors, and was later published as a book, Michael Caine said:
The eyes of the psychopath —
Anthony Hopkins as Hannibal Lecter
in Silence of the Lambs
|
However, as I think Michael began to realise, this unblinking stare is the look of the psychopath.
Cecile Paoli |
“One of the finest practitioners of this technique is Marion Brando. He denies the camera his eyes. Half the time he's looking down or away. Then suddenly he looks up, and you are absolutely fascinated by his eyes. (5)
I would like to suggest that it was not so much that Brando was hiding his eyes but that the camera had been set up on the assumption that he would be looking at the person to whom he was speaking, but, in fact, for much of his performance he was engaged in internal processing. It was this that gave his performances such lingering fascination.
References
5. Caine, Michael, Acting In Film: an actor’s take on movie making, Applause Theatre Books, New York, 1997
Yes, I found all this very fascinating. As you know, pupil dilation is another cue of which we are mostly unaware. You seem to favour dark glasses rather a lot… I wonder why that is?!
ReplyDeleteYes, pupil dilation, like blushing, is something that few can control directly but they do occur when an actor is swept away by the role they are playing. Lee Strasberg (high priest of The Method) considered it a sign of an inspired actor, but others have dismissed these autonomic responses as inconsequential, revealing nothing of an actor's ability to interpret a role. Was it in the eighties that admen started using the word "sexy" to describe inanimate objects with no normal erotic associates? I always thought that it was because they had then started to deploy cameras to study consumers eye movements when they scanned advertisements and had noted these pupil dilations — normally associated with sexual attraction — when they fell on certain things that they desired. I am also interested in the rapid eyeball oscillation that occurs when someone is pitched into a predicament beyond their preconceptions. This also occurs in sexual attraction, but when that person does not know what to do about it. As to why I often wear dark glasses — I think I will leave that to your imagination!
Delete